Consolidating Cultural Approaches to Inequality
by Adriana Abdenur and Nicole Esparza

On April 19-20th 2002, Princeton’s Department of Sociology hosted a conference titled “Race/Ethnicity, Self/Culture, and Inequality.” Michèle Lamont (Sociology, Princeton University), the conference’s organizer, put together the conference with the explicit goal of promoting greater dialogue among scholars working separately on similar topics. The meeting, generously funded by Princeton University's Department of Sociology, Center for Migration and Development, Center for International Studies, Council for the Humanities, Council on Regional Studies, Program in African-American Studies, and Program in American Studies, brought together eminent scholars from the fields of sociology, anthropology, and psychology in an attempt to formulate a more integrated disciplinary and interdisciplinary research agenda for the study of inequality. In order to complement the field’s structural focus, the conference explored the role of culture in the production and reproduction of inequality. Traditionally, the literature on inequality has considered culture through the lens of norms and beliefs, for instance in the culture of poverty thesis. The approaches represented at this conference incorporated more nuanced analyses of meaning-making and repertoires. To begin the discussion, Lamont outlined three objectives for the meeting: to integrate cultural, social psychological, and structural aspects of inequality; to explore how the self is shaped by race and class identity; and to explore the potential contributions of comparative approaches to research on the topic.

The opening panel, “Self, Culture, and Structure in Theoretical Perspective,” laid out some of the analytical tools for the study of culture and inequality. Ann Swidler (Sociology, University of California at Berkeley) outlined a framework for thinking about culture that acknowledges that culture contains multiple meanings and that people draw differently from cultural repertoires at different times. Swidler believes that culture does not match the domains of institutions closely; it is precisely the less institutionalized arenas of life that generate new cultural idioms most profusely. Drawing from her book Talk of Love (2000), she noted that, when the institution of marriage does not solve problems, people use repertoires to fill in the gaps. In tying this work to recent research on identity, Swidler argues that action schemas – a default option people employ to understand the world – are used to draw boundaries between social groups. At the same time, people are not bound by a unitary identity; they activate different identity labels according to context. By desimplifying the notion of a one-size-fits-all culture, Swidler acknowledges human agency while also recognizing that choices are limited; culture is thus both enabling and constraining.

The relationship between the self and the collective was also addressed by Mustafa Emirbayer (Sociology, University of Wisconsin-Madison). Emirbayer draws on the works of Sorokin (1957) and Parsons and Shils (1951), which distinguishes among three analytically distinct relational contexts of action (social systems, cultural systems, and personality systems). However, Emirbayer’s point of departure is neither the system nor the individual; he conceptualizes the personality system (the collective-psychological) in transpersonal or collective terms. More specifically, he focuses on emotional ties to argue that they are themselves a potential source of structure. Emirbayer stresses that expanding our understanding of racial inequality requires further elaboration of mappings (the typology of the three contexts of action) and mechanisms (recurrent causal sequences, such as identification, idealization, and projection). He calls for an expanded inventory of such mechanisms and mappings, which would yield a more generalizable approach to the role of emotions in inequality.

In contrast to these two largely theoretical approaches to inequality, Nicole Shelton (Psychology, Princeton University) offered empirical findings on the effects of discrimination from the field of social psychology. Shelton’s studies address the implications of discrimination from the target’s perspective, as well as the influence of discrimination on mental health outcomes. Her work emphasizes how variations in the meaning and salience of ethnic groups moderate minority individuals’ experience of discrimination. Her findings show that a high degree of group identification can help buffer those individuals from the adverse effects of discrimination. In addition, the studies show that endorsing a nationalist group ideology dampens the effects of discrimination. Finally, because it is less ambiguous, blatant displays of discrimination produce less stress than subtle displays. These results show how powerful perceptions of inequality are while also highlighting the malleability of those perceptions.

Discussant Dale Miller (Psychology, Princeton University) outlined how social psychologists think about the self, culture, and identity. Miller pointed out that substantial inequalities do not automatically yield substantial differences in psychological experiences. Social psychologists like Shelton try to understand these patterns by looking at the perception of discrimination. For instance, discrimination often occurs through in-group favoritism, which is rarely acknowledged by in-group members as active discrimination. Miller noted that these studies would benefit greatly from more precise definitions and accurate measures of fundamental concepts. The issue of basic definitions and language was taken up even more vociferously by John Borneman (Anthropology, Princeton University), who called for greater examination of words like emotions and discrimination and expressed skepticism at the ability of sociological theory to elaborate generalizable frameworks in the study of culture and inequality.

These problems of definition have been tackled in many ways by scholars of race. In the second panel, “Self, Racism, and Intra-Racial Differences,” the presenters addressed various types of settings in which race impacts the construction of the self. Claude Steele (Psychology, Stanford University) drew on a general stereotype threat theory that posits that as people engage in a social setting they sometimes face the possibility of being devalued based on one or more identities. Cues indicating this possibility can trigger an appraisal process in which Steele has identified as a major tension: When the threat of devaluation emerges in a setting to which the individual aspires to belong, the impulse to be vigilant about threats conflicts with the impulse to overlook those same cues. This tension can become a long-term characteristic of the individual’s experience in that particular setting. Steele illustrated this theory by summarizing several experiments, one of which tested minority students’ perceptions of bias in instructor evaluations. His conclusion that ambiguity isolates the individual from the feedback, leading to a loss of motivation, resonates with Shelton’s findings that ambiguity in displays of discrimination produces adverse mental health outcomes.

Whereas Steele studied a variety of social settings, anthropologist John Jackson (Harvard Society of Fellows) focused on a specific setting to illustrate the richness and fluidity of symbolic meanings. Jackson drew on his recently published book Harlemworld (2001), an ethnography of class relationships among Harlem residents. Jackson explained that gentrification has prompted Harlemites to draw on different discourses to justify who belongs in the neighborhood. For example, the hip-hop generation of Harlemites has constructed a fantasy Harlem -- a Disneyfied “Harlemworld” -- that is completely abstracted from the realities of the flesh-and-blood neighborhood. In hip-hop songs, Harlemworld is a homogenous landscape of mansions, limos, and private jets. This mythical reinterpretation -- an act of rejection and renewal -- shows how local identities are reshaped through cultural production.

Religion is often a key moderator in this process. Nancy DiTomaso (Business School, Rutgers University) talked about her interview-based research exploring attitudes towards racial inequality among working class and middle class people by focusing on their interplay with political orientation and religious beliefs. DiTomaso sees the construction of meaning about both identity and politics as cultural productions that are mediated by religion. She has found a correlation between conservative political opinions of race relations and social welfare topics and the degree of religious identity.

A more longitudinal approach was seen in the presentation by Frank Furstenberg (Sociology, University of Pennsylvania) who provided an overview of his thirty-year study on the impact of teenage pregnancy. Furstenberg conducted three waves of interviews beginning in the mid-sixties and found that by the mid-nineties the differences between teenage mothers and their peers had diminished considerably. He uses these results to critique the popular notion that teen mothers become chronically welfare-dependent. Furstenberg has found that many of the women in his study eventually went back to school and successfully entered the job market. His study contradicts the culture of poverty literature by illustrating how culture and structure interact: rather than assuming that a specific set of values yields consistently negative outcomes, he shows that many of these women, with their families’ support, were able to overcome structural barriers through resilience and strategy.

In his discussion of Furstenberg’s paper, Mitchell Duneier (Sociology, University of Wisconsin-Madison and UCSB) framed those findings explicitly in terms of cultural repertoires, highlighting that the women in the study led their lives in a continuing dialogue with the stereotypes attached to them. Likewise, Duneier called for the incorporation of social psychological approaches into studies of inequality. He pointed out that Steele’s paper shows that, while stereotypes are powerful, even small encouragements can lead to substantially more positive outcomes. Duneier also addressed the work of Jackson and DiTomaso in terms of cultural production, noting that macro-structural change in Harlem can be seen in the neighborhood’s changing identity and that political leanings are strongly influenced by religious beliefs.

The variety of methods used to study race also characterizes approaches in class and in part reflects competing definitions of class. In the third panel, “Self and Class Differences,” class was operationalized either in terms of lifestyles or in terms of occupation. Annette Lareau (Sociology, Temple University) presented her ethnographic work on the parenting practices of middle-class mothers. Lareau notes that scholars of inequality tend to focus on unequal distribution of resources; she focuses instead on the strategies that people employ when using those resources to solve day-to-day problems. She also points out that sometimes studies of inequality focus too narrowly on race at the expense of class; she therefore tackles these two dimensions simultaneously. More specifically, she argues that, while black middle-class families often encounter race-specific problems, they draw on generic middle-class resources to solve them. Her vivid account of the Marshall family shows how black middle-class mothers transmit to their children the sense of entitlement and negotiation skills that are typical of middle-class, while also transmitting the strategies needed to navigate situations of potential discrimination.

This lifestyle-based definition of class is also central to Maria Kefalas’ (Sociology, St. Joseph’s University) study of housewives in a predominately white working-class suburb of Chicago. Kefalas focuses on the relationship between consumption and post-industrial cultural labor, arguing that home ownership and care in this setting are fundamental to definitions of self-worth and mutual moral pronouncements. The houseproud women, some of whom get down on their knees to trim the grass with scissors, engage in a social performance that is simultaneously a genuine process of identity construction. The house becomes an extension of the self, and the housewives use cleanliness and domesticity to uphold their family’s respectability. Kefalas interprets this symbolic work as a symptom of the economic insecurity of working-class families aspiring to middle-class status. Her work resonates with Michèle Lamont’s work on boundaries in that it illustrates how social groups sometimes draw on moral criteria to assert their dignity.

In contrast to Lareau and Kefalas’ ethnographic work, David Grusky (Sociology, Cornell University) employs a quantitative approach to class analysis by tackling the postmodern view that classes are losing their historical role as pivotal axes of social organization. In his work with Kim Weeden, he argues that there is inadequate evidence to support this perspective and proposes that students of class focus on a more disaggregated level, that of relatively small clusters of detailed occupations. At the level of these disaggregated “micro-classes,” the effects of class are stronger because the division of labor gives rise to deeply institutionalized social groups. According to this perspective, social closure operates not at the aggregate level but rather at the disaggregate level of occupations. Grusky and Weeden compared the effects of class at those two levels of analysis and found a decline in differences between the effects of conventional class categories but great stability in class structuration at the micro-class level. Their study is particularly original because it allows them to identify what aspects of class structuration (identity, lifestyle, political, attitudes, etc.) change at the big-class and micro-class levels.

Discussant Carolyn Rouse (Anthropology, Princeton University) commented on the importance of Kefalas’ work for looking at consumption as a means of identity construction. Additionally, she noted that work on inequality should also look at institutions as contributing to social mobility. Likewise, Paul DiMaggio (Sociology, Princeton University) noted that Lareau’s account of the Marshall family is precisely what Swidler means when she says that culture differs in the availability and use of toolkits. DiMaggio pointed out that the intimate connection that people make between their homes and their selves reflects the important role of emotions in constituting identity that Emirbayer elaborates in his paper. The comments by Rouse and DiMaggio highlighted the common quest -- seen in all three presentations -- to establish where and how, both culturally and structurally, social boundaries develop.

Boundary formation was explored within the context of immigration by Mary Waters (Sociology, Harvard University) in the opening presentation of the third panel, “Immigration and Culture: European and American Perspectives.” In her collaborative work with Philip Kasinitz and John Mollenkopf on the second generation in New York City, Waters assesses how young adults construct their identities vis-à-vis both their parents’ original cultural practices and the complex mainstream cultural landscape of New York. Her presentation stressed the role of local immigration history in shaping the institutions that facilitate integration. In particular, she cited past waves of white immigration and the legacy of the civil rights movement in rendering New York immigrant institutions as uniquely accommodating. These ideas are bolstered by the research project’s multi-method, comparative study of six immigrant groups. Waters concluded by calling for a reassessment of the term generation and for greater attention to the complexity of “mainstream” culture.

Yet another dimension of generation -- the increasingly transnational aspects of immigration -- was analyzed by Peggy Levitt (Sociology Wellesley College and Harvard University). Levitt drew on her study of Pakistani immigrants living in Massachusetts and their families back home to show how immigrant groups can selectively assimilate by drawing on two national cultural repertoires. In the case of Pakistanis, this transnational array of cultural tools leads immigrants to selectively adapt their practices and institutions to the local context. While religion plays a key role in the assimilation process, class shapes institutional templates and the nature of immigrants’ contact with other groups. These highly educated Pakistani immigrants work alongside native-born Americans yet seldom interact with them in social settings. The immigrants maintain those cultural practices that are most easily translatable to the local cultural context and either discard or modify those that seem inconsistent with the perceived advantages of the local environment. This selectivity is facilitated by the malleability of “Pakistaniness,” which is itself an identity in flux. Levitt highlights the need for reference to the country of origin, since immigration often entails cultural change there as well as in the country of destination.

The issue of second generation was addressed from a cross-national comparative perspective by Richard Alba (Sociology, SUNY-Albany). By examining the institutions that define the mainstream and therefore shape assimilation in the US, France, and Germany, Alba compares processes of boundary blurring with those of boundary crossing. His key idea is that boundaries separate the mainstream from the minority, and that their salience and permeability shape the process of immigrant incorporation. He argues that France and Germany have more crystallized mainstreams because their histories’ of state formation and major religious conflicts yielded less permeable boundaries. In the US, on the other hand, boundaries have shifted to accommodate more groups, particularly Jews and Catholics. Alba’s work, like that of Waters and Levitt, suggests that comparative international approaches to immigration would produce much more sophisticated understandings of boundary work across settings and generations.

Both discussants for this panel, Mario Small (Sociology and Office of Population Research, Princeton University) and Louise Lamphere (Anthropology, University of New Mexico and Russell Sage Foundation), noted that the cultural and the political are often conflated; for instance Small wondered whether the differences in assimilation seen in Alba’s work were the result of different national cultures or different political systems. While the cultural and the political obviously interact at a number of different levels, distinguishing them analytically would produce a clearer picture of how historical legacies and institutional frameworks impact patterns of assimilation.

Analytical distinctions were the main preoccupation of the concluding panel, which brought together scholars from the three fields represented in the conference to discuss to the overarching theme of comparative research. Rogers Brubaker (Sociology, UCLA) warned against imposing prefabricated schemas when studying inequality and culture comparatively. His principal concern regarding cross-national research is the use of aggregated units of analysis like the nation. Brubaker drew a distinction between comparative research and comparativism, arguing that studies of the self and culture are intrinsically comparative. He proposes that researchers let go of their presumptions about collective boundaries and instead identify important loci of action over the course of research.

A similar concern was voiced by Adrian Favell (Sociology, UCLA), who noted that the rise of the nation state has led sociologists to erroneously carve up the world into self-contained social systems. In fact, noted Favell, boundaries are porous and cultural systems interpenetrate, particularly in this age of massive immigration. Moreover, much of present-day cross-national research -- particularly that focusing on the US and Western Europe -- tends to err on the side of exceptionalism. Favell is especially concerned with the habit of drawing comparisons between the US and individual Western European countries; a more appropriate point of departure would be to compare the United States with Europe as a whole.

Comparative research in each field would benefit from incorporation of work done in other areas. For instance, Shibonu Kitayama (Psychology, University of Kyoto) showed how different cultural environments in Japan and the United States produce differences in cognitive styles. One of his key points is that people will focus on and feel most emotional about those things that are emphasized within their culture. The extent to which people are engaged in patterns of collective action influence how they perceive the self and human agency. Kitayama suggests that psychology may be far more social in origin than psychologists have so far assumed and that psychological processes are far more real than social scientists have so far imagined.

Despite the strong argument for interdisciplinary dialogue supported by Kitayama’s work, persistent differences in definitions create obstacles for this type of collaboration. Vincent Crapanzano (Anthropology, CUNY) reiterated John Borneman’s skepticism about the language of inequality. Crapanzano noted that the category of race does not translate smoothly across national contexts. Like Brubaker, he criticized the use of such categories on the grounds that they are products of researchers’ presuppositions. Crapanzano insists that scholars resist the temptation to simplify excessively, stressing that discussions at the meta-level are necessary for grasping the complexity of social life. He thus cautions against overextending comparisons across contexts when the basic communicative exchanges imply nuanced yet vastly different interpretative frameworks.

John Bowen (Anthropology, Washington University) proposed a way to tackle these conceptual problems by arguing that comparative research would benefit from the further consideration of three key concepts: distribution, trajectories, and repertoires. Bowen criticizes the emphasis that social scientists place on differences between means and their negligence of within-group variation, noting for instance that often the most enduring and vicious debates are internal to groups. He thus proposes a shift from means and groups to trajectories, which allow us to think more deeply about discontinuous variables. Some of this variability is due to the different accessibility of repertoires, which generate a multitude of interpretations.

The extent to which these discussions will foster interdisciplinary research remains to be seen. The presentations outlined above showcased the wealth of theoretical frameworks and methodologies available to researchers of inequality, yet they also revealed disciplinary fault lines that often obstruct dialogue between researchers interested in the same topic. Gaps along another axis – that between theory and empiricism – also persist even within disciplines and could profit from refinement of the language and measurements used to study the various facets of inequality. At the same time, the conference highlighted certain key themes that seem particularly promising in the effort to further consolidate cultural approaches to inequality. In general, serious and systematic consideration of perceptions of inequality is needed for a more sophisticated understanding of how the self, context, and collectivities interact, including boundary-related processes. For instance, several presentations explored from different angles how ambiguity in situations of potential discrimination can influence outcomes that are both internal and external to the individual. Likewise, perceptions of mobility are powerful determinants of behavior and yet are highly susceptible to change. A related theme, the role of emotions in inequality, has also been given short rift by sociologists and yet offers ample opportunities for exploring patterns of individual and collective behavior. Finally, all of these interdisciplinary bridges would benefit from further comparative research exploring the multiple ways in which culture shapes the production and reproduction of inequality.

* A version of this report is being published in the American Sociological Association's Culture newsletter.

People Job Candidates Programs Links Archive Home